TO WHAT EXTENT DO GENES
CAUSE AUTISM?
The heritability of autism is a source of controversy
about the causes of
autism. Though it is agreed that there is a genetic susceptibility
to autism, disagreements arise
over the whether the condition is genetically determined and therefore
inevitable, or is triggered by factors in the environment. The controversy
is made more difficult by the broad spectrum of phenotypes labeled
"autism", ranging from near total disability to mild social
difficulties.
Identical twin studies put autism's heritability
in a range between 0.36 and 0.957, with concordance for a broader
phenotype usually found at the higher end of the range.[1] Autism
concordance in siblings and fraternal twins is anywhere between
0 and 23.5%. This is more likely 2–4% for classic autism and 10–20%
for a broader spectrum. Assuming a general-population prevalence
of 0.1%, the risk of classic autism in siblings is 20- to 40-fold
that of the general population.
Researchers usually note that autism is among
the most heritable of all neurological conditions, even among the
more than 90% of cases not associated with known genetic diseases
such as fragile X syndrome
or muscular dystrophy.[2][3]
Twin studies
Twin studies are a helpful tool in determining
the heritability of disorders and low-prevalence human traits in
general. They involve determining concordance of characteristics
between identical (monozygotic or MZ) twins and between fraternal
(dizygotic or DZ) twins. Possible problems of twin studies are:
(1) errors in diagnosis of monozygocity, and (2) the assumption
that social environment sharing by DZ twins is equivalent to that
of MZ twins.
A condition that is environmentally caused without
genetic involvement would yield a concordance for MZ twins equal
to the concordance found for DZ twins. In contrast, a condition
that is completely genetic in origin would theoretically yield a
concordance of 100% for MZ pairs and usually much less for DZ pairs
depending on factors such as the number of genes involved and assortative
mating.
An example of a condition that appears to have
very little if any genetic influence is irritable bowel syndrome
(IBS), with a concordance of 28% vs. 27% for MZ and DZ pairs respectively.[4]
An example of a human characteristics that is extremely heritable
is eye color, with a concordance of 98% for MZ pairs and 7–49% for
DZ pairs depending on age.[5]
Notable twin studies have attempted to shed light
on the heritability of autism.
A small scale study in 1977 was the first of its
kind to look into the heritability of autism. It involved 10 DZ
and 11 MZ pairs in which at least one twin in each pair showed infantile
autism. It found a concordance of 36% in MZ twins compared to 0%
for DZ twins. Concordance of "cognitive abnormalities"
was 82% in MZ pairs and 10% for DZ pairs. In 12 of the 17 pairs
discordant for autism, a biological hazard was believed to be associated
with the condition.[6]
A 1979 case report discussed a pair of identical
twins concordant for autism. The twins developed similarly until
the age of 4, when one of them spontaneously improved. The other
twin, who had suffered infrequent seizures,
remained autistic. The report noted that genetic factors were not
"all important" in the development of the twins.[7]
In 1985, a study of twins enrolled with the UCLA
Registry for Genetic Studies found a concordance of 95.7% for autism
in 23 pairs of MZ twins, and 23.5% for 17 DZ twins.[8]
In a 1989 study, Nordic countries were screened
for cases of autism. Eleven pairs of MZ twins and 10 of DZ twins
were examined. Concordance of autism was found to be 91% in MZ and
0% in DZ pairs. The concordances for "cognitive disorder"
were 91% and 30% respectively. In most of the pairs discordant for
autism, the autistic twin had more perinatal stress.[9]
A British twin sample was reexamined in 1995 and
a 60% concordance was found for autism in MZ twins vs. 0% concordance
for DZ. It also found 92% concordance for a broader spectrum in
MZ vs. 10% for DZ. The study concluded that "obstetric hazards
usually appear to be consequences of genetically influenced abnormal
development, rather than independent aetiological factors."[10]
A 1999 study looked at social cognitive skills
in general-population children and adolescents. It found "poorer
social cognition in males", and a heritability of 0.68 with
higher genetic influence in younger twins.[11]
In 2000, a study looked at reciprocal social behavior
in general-population identical twins. It found a concordance of
73% for MZ, i.e. "highly heritable", and 37% for DZ pairs.[12]
A 2004 study looked at 16 MZ twins and found a
concordance of 43.75% for "strictly defined autism". Neuroanatomical
differences (discordant cerebellar white and grey matter volumes)
between discordant twins were found. The abstract notes that in
previous studies 75% of the non-autistic twins displayed the broader
phenotype.[13]
Another 2004 study examined whether the characteristic
symptoms of autism (impaired social interaction, communication deficits,
and repetitive
behaviors) show decreased variance of symptoms among monozygotic
twins compared to siblings in a sample of 16 families. The study
demonstrated significant aggregation of symptoms in twins. It also
concluded that "the levels of clinical features seen in autism
may be a result of mainly independent genetic traits."[14]
An English twin study in 2006 found high heritability
for autistic traits in a large group of 3,400 pairs of twins.[15]
One critic of the pre-2006 twin studies said that
they were too small and their results can be plausibly explained
on non-genetic grounds.[16]
Sibling studies
The importance of sibling studies lies in contrasting
their results to those of fraternal (DZ) twin studies, plus their
sample sizes can be much larger. Environment sharing by siblings
is presumably different enough to that of DZ twins to shed some
light on the magnitude of environmental influence. This should even
be true to some extent regarding the prenatal environment. Unfortunately
DZ twin study findings have yielded a very large range of variance
and are error prone because of the apparent low concordance and
the fact that they typically look at a small number of DZ pairs.
For example, in studies involving 10 DZ pairs, a concordance below
10% would be impossible to determine precisely.
A study of 99 autistic probands which found a
2.9% concordance for autism in siblings, and between 12.4% and 20.4%
concordance for a "lesser variant" of autism.[17]
A study of 31 siblings of autistic children, 32
siblings of children with developmental delay, and 32 controls.
It found that the siblings of autistic children, as a group, "showed
superior spatial and verbal span, but a greater than expected number
performed poorly on the set-shifting, planning, and verbal fluency
tasks."[18]
A 2005 Danish study looked at "data from
the Danish Psychiatric Central Register and the Danish Civil Registration
System to study some risk factors of autism, including place of
birth, parental place of birth, parental age, family history of
psychiatric disorders, and paternal identity." It found an
overall prevalence rate of roughly 0.08%. Prevalence of autism in
siblings of autistic children was found to be 1.76%. Prevalence
of autism among siblings of children with Asperger's syndrome or
PDD was found to be 1.04%. The risk was twice as high if the mother
had been diagnosed with a psychiatric disorder. The study also found
that "the risk of autism was associated with increasing degree
of urbanisation of the child's place of birth and with increasing
paternal, but not maternal, age."[19]
A study in 2007 looked at a database containing pedigrees of 86
families with two or more autistic children and found that 42 of
the third-born male children showed autistic symptoms, suggesting
that parents had a 50% chance of passing on a mutation to their
offspring. The mathematical models suggest that about 50% of autistic
cases are caused by spontaneous mutations. The simplest model was
to divide parents into two risk classes depending on whether the
parent carries a pre-existing mutation that causes autism; it suggested
that about a quarter of autistic children have inherited a copy
number variation from their parents.[20]
Other family studies
A 1994 looked at the personalities of parents
of autistic children, using parents of children with Down's syndrome
as controls. Using standardized tests it was found that parents
of autistic children were "more aloof, untactful and unresponsive."[21]
A 1997 study found higher rates of social and communication deficits
and stereotyped behaviors in families with multiple-incidence autism.[22]
Autism was found to occur more often in families of physicists,
engineers and scientists.[23] Other studies have yielded similar
results.[24][25] Findings of this nature have led to the coinage
of the term "geek syndrome".[26]
A 2001 study of brothers and parents of autistic boys looked into
the phenotype in terms of one current cognitive theory of autism.
The study raised the possibility that the broader autism phenotype
may include a "cognitive style" (weak central coherence)
that can confer information-processing advantages.[27]
A study in 2005 showed a positive correlation between repetitive
behaviors in autistic individuals and obsessive-compulsive behaviors
in parents.[28] Another 2005 study focused on sub-threashold autistic
traits in the general population. It found that correlation for
social impairment or competence between parents and their children
and between spouses is about 0.4.[29]
A 2005 report examined the family psychiatric
history of 58 subjects with Asperger's syndrome (AS) diagnosed according
to DSM-IV criteria. Three (5%) had first-degree relatives with Aspergers syndrome.
Nine (19%) had a family history of schizophrenia. Thirty five (60%)
had a family history of depression. Out of 64 siblings, 4 (6.25%)
were diagnosed with Aspergers syndrome.[30]
Twinning risk
It has been suggested that the twinning process
itself is a risk factor in the development of autism, presumably
due to perinatal factors.[31] However, three large-scale epidemiological
studies have refuted this idea.[32][33]
Phenocopies
Evidence has mounted indicating that clinical
pictures that look like autism (phenocopies) may not be due to the
same genetic liability. Examples are congenital blindness,[34] profound
institutional privation,[35][36] and a number of conditions related
to intellectual
disability.[37]
Fragile-X syndrome, Rett
Syndrome and tuberous
sclerosis are well-known causes of autism-like symptoms.
Proposed models
Twin and family studies show that autism is a
highly heritable condition, but they have left many questions for
researchers, most notably
Why is fraternal twin concordance so low considering
that identical twin concordance is high?
Why are parents of autistic children typically non-autistic?
Which factors could be involved in the failure to find a 100% concordance
in identical twins?
Is profound intellectual disability a characteristic of the genotype
or something totally independent?
Some researchers have speculated that what we currently refer to
as "autism" may be a catch-all description for many yet
unknown conditions with different genetic and/or environmental etiologies.
This would appear to make the effort to find a genotype model a
lot more difficult, and perhaps even pointless. Nevertheless, a
number of genetic models have been proposed to try to explain the
results of twin and sibling studies.
Mendelian
The original Mendelian model tried to explain
observations using distinct genes existing in clearly dominant or
recessive alleles. That would imply a recessive "autism gene"
inherited from each of the parents. This kind of model is clearly
too simple:[38]
It indicates that a sibling of an autistic individual should have
25% risk of having the autistic genotype, which is inconsistent
with fraternal twin and sibling study results.
It would require several characteristic features of autism to be
caused by a single allele at a single locus.
Further considerations for the 'autism gene model' of also show
contradictory implications:
(a) only a small number of cases can be clearly
linked to a possible genetic cause and these are often allele deletions;
(b) the majority of patients with autism do not marry and do not
have offspring which should result in a decreased incidence of the
presumed gene in the general population.
(c) the incidence of autism in the population has been increasing
instead, making the likelihood of a single genetic cause extremely
remote.
Mendel's later work and work based on it introduced polygenic inheritance,
but taking into account linkage of genes required understanding
where they were located - elucidating the role of the chromosomes.
Multigene
Reduced risk to relatives of probands and identical/fraternal
twin ratios indicate that a multigene model is more likely to account
for the autistic genotype. That is, at least two alleles would be
involved, and most likely three to five. Researchers have suggested
models of 15 and even up to 100 genes.
The fraternal twin results found by Ritvo et al (1985)[8] and the
broader phenotype results of Bolton et al (1994)[17] suggest that
a 2-gene model is plausible. Kolevzon et al (2004) proposed that
the 3 characteristic symptoms of autism may be the result of 3 different
alleles. Data supports the multiple-locus hypothesis
and also that a 3-loci model is the best fit.[39] Risch et al (1999)
found results most compatible with a large number of loci (>=
15).[40]
Given the significant prevalence of autism, perhaps
0.1% for classic autism and at least 0.6% for a broader spectrum,
a multigene model has important implications. Since intelligence
appears to be independent of the recognized characteristic symptoms
of autism (and the diagnostic criteria) it is likely that many individuals
are very autistic yet highly functional, allowing them to escape
a diagnosis
altogether. So the prevalence of the autistic genotype may be considerably
higher than thought. And if multiple alleles are part of the genotype,
then each allele must have relatively high prevalence in the general
population.
Two family types
In this model most families fall into two types:
in the majority, sons have a low risk of autism, but in a small
minority their risk is near 50%. In the low-risk families, sporadic
autism is mainly caused by spontaneous mutation with poor penetrance
in daughters and high penetrance in sons. The high-risk families
come from (mostly female) children who carry a new causative mutation
but are unaffected and transmit the dominant mutation to grandchildren.[20]
Epigenetic
A number of epigenetic models of autism have been
proposed as have several genetic imprinting models.[41][42] These
are suggested by the occurrence of autism in individuals with fragile
X syndrome, which arises from epigenetic mutations, and with Rett
Syndrome, which involves epigenetic regulatory factors. An epigenetic
model would help explain why standard genetic screening strategies
have so much difficulty with autism.[43]
Candidate gene loci
A number of alleles have been shown to have strong
linkage to the autism phenotype. In many cases the findings are
inconclusive, with some studies showing no linkage. Alleles linked
so far strongly support the assertion that there is a large number
of genotypes that are manifested as the autism phenotype. At least
some of the alleles associated with autism are fairly prevalent
in the general population, which indicates they are not rare pathogenic
mutations. This also presents some challenges in identifying all
the rare allele combinations involved in the etiology of autism.
17q11.2 region, SERT (SLC6A4) locus – This gene locus has been
associated with rigid-compulsive behaviors. Notably, it has also
been associated with depression but only as a result of social adversity,
although other studies have found no link.[44] Significant linkage
in families with only affected males has been shown.[45][46] Researchers
have also suggested that the gene contributes to hyperserotonemia.[47]
GABA receptor subunit genes – GABA is the primary inhibitory neurotransmitter
of the human brain. Ma et al (2005) concluded that GABRA4 is involved
in the etiology of autism, and that it potentially increases autism
risk through interaction with GABRB1.[48] The GABRB3 gene has been
associated with savant skills.[49] The GABRB3 gene deficient mouse
has been proposed as a model of Autism Spectrum Disorder.[50]
Engrailed 2 (EN2) – Engrailed 2 is believed to be associated with
cerebellar development. Benayed et al (2005) estimate that this
gene contributes to as many as 40% of Autism Spectrum Disorder cases, about twice the
prevalence of the general population.[51] But at least one study
has found no association.[52]
3q25-27 region – A number of studies have shown a significant linkage
of autism and Asperger's syndrome with this locus.[53][54] The most
prominent markers are in the vicinity of D3S3715 and D3S3037.[55]
7q21-q36 region, REELIN (RELN) – In adults, Reelin glycoprotein
is believed to be involved in memory formation, neurotransmission,
and synaptic plasticity. A number of studies have shown an association
between the REELIN gene and autism,[56][57] but a couple of studies
were unable to duplicate linkage findings.[58]
SLC25A12 – This gene, located in chromosome 2q31, encodes the mitochondrial
aspartate/glutamate carrier (AGC1). It has been found to have a
significant linkage to autism in some studies,[59][60] but linkage
was not replicated in others,[61] and a 2007 study found no compelling
evidence of an association of any mitochondrial haplogroup in autism.[62]
HOXA1 and HOXB1 – A link has been found between HOX genes and the
development of the embryonic brain stem. In particular, two genes,
HOXA1 and HOXB1, in transgenic 'knockout' mice, engineered so that
these genes were absent from the genomes of the mice in question,
exhibited very specific brain stem developmental differences from
the norm, which were directly comparable to the brain stem differences
discovered in a human brain stem originating from a diagnosed autistic
patient.[63]
Conciatori et al (2004) found an association of HOXA1 with increased
head circumference.[64] A number of studies have found no association
with autism.[65][66][67] The possibility remains that single allelic
variants of the HOXA1 gene are insufficient alone to trigger the
developmental events in the embryo now associated with autism spectrum conditions. Tischfield et al published a paper which suggests
that because HOXA1 is implicated in a wide range of developmental
mechanisms, a model involving multiple allelic variants of HOXA1
in particular may provide useful insights into the heritability
mechanisms involved.[68] Additionally, Ingram et al alighted upon
additional possibilities in this arena.[69] Transgenic mouse studies
indicate that there is redundancy spread across HOX genes that complicate
the issue, and that complex interactions between these genes could
play a role in determining whether or not a person inheriting the
requisite combinations manifests an autistic spectum condition[70]—transgenic
mice with mutations in both HOXA1 and HOXB1 exhibit far more profound
developmental anomalies than those in which only one of the genes
differs from the conserved 'norm'.
In Rodier's original work, teratogens are considered to play a
part in addition, and that the possibility remains open for a range
of teratogens to interact with the mechanisms controlled by these
genes unfavourably (this has already been demonstrated using valproic
acid, a known teratogen, in the mouse model).
PRKCB1 – Philippi et al (2005) found a strong association between
this gene and autism. This is a recent finding that needs to be
replicated.[71]
FOXP2 – The FOXP2 gene is of interest because it is known to be
associated with developmental language and speech deficits. An association
to autism appears to be elusive, nonetheless.[72][73]
UBE3A – The UBE3A gene has been associated with Angelman syndrome.
Samaco et al (2005) suggest reduced expression of UBE3A in autism,
as is the case in Rett syndrome.[74] In any case, it appears that
the role of UBE3A is limited.
Shank3/ProSAP2, 22q13 and Neuroligins – The gene called SHANK3
(also designated ProSAP2) regulates the structural organization
of neurotranmsitter receptors in post-synaptic dendritic spines
making it a key element in chemical binding crucial to nerve cell
communication.[75] SHANK3 is also a binding partner of chromosome
22q13 (i.e. a specific section of Chromosome 22) and neuroligin
proteins; deletions and mutations of SHANK3, 22q13 (i.e. a specific
section of Chromosome 22) and genes encoding neuroligins have been
found in some people with Autism Spectrum Disorders.[76]
Mutations in the SHANK3 gene have been strongly associated with
the Autism Spectrum Disorders. If the SHANK3 gene is not adequately
passed to a child from the parent (haploinsufficiency) there will
possibly be significant neurological changes that are associated
with yet another gene, 22q13, which interacts with SHANK3. Alteration
or deletion of either will effect changes in the other.[76]
A deletion of a single copy of a gene on chromosome 22q13 has been
correlated with global developmental delay, severely delayed speech
or social communication disorders and moderate to profound delay
of cognitive abilities. Behavior is described as "autistic-like"
and includes high tolerance to pain and habitual chewing or mouthing[76]
(see also 22q13 deletion syndrome). This appears to be connected
to the fact that signal transmission between nerve cells is altered
with the absence of 22q13.
SHANK3 proteins also interact with neuroligins at the synapses
of the brain further complicating the widespread effects of changes
at the genetic level and beyond.[77]
Neuroligin is a cell surface protein (homologous to acetylcholinesterase
and other esterases) that binds to synaptic membranes.[78] Neuroligins
organize postsynaptic membranes that function to transmit nerve
cell messages (excitatory) and stop those transmissions (inhibitory);[79]
In this way, neuroligins help to ensure signal transitions between
nerve cells. Neuroligins are also regulate the maturation of synapses
and ensure there are sufficient receptor proteins on the synaptic
membrane.
Mice with a neuroligin-3 mutation exhibit poor social skills but
increased intelligence.[80] Though not present in all individuals
with autism, these mutations hold potential to illustrate some of
the genetic components of spectrum disorders.[77]
MET – The MET gene (MET receptor tyrosine kinase gene) linked to
brain development, regulation of the immune system, and repair of
the gastrointestinal system, has been linked to autism. This MET
gene codes for a protein that relays signals that turn on a cell’s
internal machinery. Impairing the receptor’s signaling interferes
with neuron migration and disrupts neuronal growth in the cerebral
cortex and similarly shrinks the cerebellum—abnormalities also seen
in autism.[81]
It is also known to play a key role in both normal and abnormal
development, such as cancer metastases (hence the name MET). A mutation
of the gene, rendering it less active, has been found to be common
amongst children with autism.[81] Mutation in the MET gene demonstrably
raises risk of autism by 2.27 times.[82]
neurexin 1 – In February 2007, researchers in the Autism Genome
Project (an international research team composed of 137 scientists
in 50 institutions) reported possible implications in aberrations
of a brain-development gene called neurexin 1 (located on chromosome
11) as a cause of some cases of autism.[83] Linkage analysis was
performed on DNA from 1,181 families in what was the largest-scale
genome scan conducted in autism research at the time.
The objective of the study was to locate specific brain cells involved
in autism to find regions in the genome linked to autism susceptibility
genes. The focus of the research was copy number variations (CNVs),
extra or missing parts of genes. Each person does not actually have
just an exact copy of genes from each parent. Each person also has
occasional multiple copies of one or more genes or some genes are
missing altogether. The research team attempted to locate CNVs when
they scanned the DNA.
Neurexin 1 is one of the genes that may be involved in communication
between nerve cells (neurons). Neurexin 1 and other genes like it
are very important in determining how the brain is connected from
cell to cell, and in the chemical transmission of information between
nerve cells. These genes are particularly active very early in brain
development, either in utero or in the first months or couple of
years of life. In some families their autistic child had only one
copy of the neurexin 1 gene.
Besides actually locating yet another possible genetic influence
(the findings were statistically insignificant), the research also
reinforced the theory that autism involves many forms of genetic
variations.
GSTP1 – A 2007 study suggested that the GSTP1*A haplotype of the
glutathione S-transferase P1 gene (GSTP1) acts in the mother during
pregnancy and increases the likelihood of autism in the child.[84]
Others – There is a large number of other candidate loci which
either should be looked at or have been shown to be promising. Several
genome-wide scans have been performed identifying markers across
many chromosomes.[85][86][87]
A few examples of loci that have been studied are the 17q21 region, the 3p24-26 locus,[85] PTEN,[88] and 15q11-q13.[49]
Other possible candidates include:
SLC6A2 (Social phobia)
FMR1 (Fragile-X)
5-HT-1Dbeta (OCD)
7q11.23 (William's syndrome, language impairment)
4q34-35, 5q35.2-35.3, 17q25 (Tourette
syndrome)
2q24.1-31.1 (Intelligence)
6p25.3-22.3 (Verbal IQ)
22q11.2 (Visio-Spatial IQ)
References
1 Twin studies (concordance in brackets):
(0.8–1) Ciaranello, Roland D. M.DThe Neurobiology of Infantile Autism
(0.8) Kallen, Ronald J. M.D CDC Reports a higher than expected prevalence
of autism in Brick Township
(0.91–0.93) Dawson, Geraldine Ph. DWritten testimony Public Health
Subcommittee, United States Senate
(0.9) Lang, Leslie H.Study points to chromosome site of autism gene
(0.6–0.92) Muhle R, Trentacoste SV, Rapin I (2004). "The genetics
of autism". Pediatrics 113 (5): e472–86. PMID 15121991.
(0.6–0.8) Kurita H (2001). "[Current status of autism studies]"
(in Japanese). Seishin shinkeigaku zasshi = Psychiatria et neurologia
Japonica 103 (1): 64–75. PMID 11383012.
2 Folstein SE, Rosen-Sheidley B (2001). "Genetics of autism:
complex aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder". Nat Rev Genet
2 (12): 943–55. doi:10.1038/35103559. PMID 11733747.
3 Muhle R, Trentacoste SV, Rapin I (2004). "The genetics of
autism". Pediatrics 113 (5): e472–86. PMID 15121991.
4 Mohammed I, Cherkas LF, Riley SA, Spector TD, Trudgill NJ (2005).
"Genetic influences in irritable bowel syndrome: a twin study".
Am. J. Gastroenterol. 100 (6): 1340–4. doi:10.1111/j.1572-0241.2005.41700.x.
PMID 15929767.
5 Bito LZ, Matheny A, Cruickshanks KJ, Nondahl DM, Carino OB (1997).
"Eye color changes past early childhood. The Louisville Twin
Study". Arch. Ophthalmol. 115 (5): 659–63. PMID 9152135.
6 Folstein S, Rutter M (1977). "Infantile autism: a genetic
study of 21 twin pairs". Journal of child psychology and psychiatry,
and allied disciplines 18 (4): 297–321. PMID 562353.
7 Wessels WH, Pompe van Meerdervoort M (1979). "Monozygotic
twins with early infantile autism. A case report". S. Afr.
Med. J. 55 (23): 955–7. PMID 572995.
8 Ritvo ER, Freeman BJ, Mason-Brothers A, Mo A, Ritvo AM (1985).
"Concordance for the syndrome of autism in 40 pairs of afflicted
twins". The American journal of psychiatry 142 (1): 74–7. PMID
4038442.
9 Steffenburg S, Gillberg C, Hellgren L, et al (1989). "A twin
study of autism in Denmark, Finland, Iceland, Norway and Sweden".
Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines
30 (3): 405–16. PMID 2745591.
10 Bailey A, Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, et al (1995). "Autism
as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study".
Psychological medicine 25 (1): 63–77. PMID 7792363.
^ Scourfield J, Martin N, Lewis G, McGuffin P (1999). "Heritability
of social cognitive skills in children and adolescents". The
British journal of psychiatry : the journal of mental science 175:
559–64. PMID 10789354.
^ Constantino JN, Todd RD (2000). "Genetic structure of reciprocal
social behavior". The American journal of psychiatry 157 (12):
2043–5. PMID 11097975.
^ Kates WR, Burnette CP, Eliez S, et al (2004). "Neuroanatomic
variation in monozygotic twin pairs discordant for the narrow phenotype
for autism". The American journal of psychiatry 161 (3): 539–46.
PMID 14992981.
^ Kolevzon A, Smith CJ, Schmeidler J, Buxbaum JD, Silverman JM (2004).
"Familial symptom domains in monozygotic siblings with autism".
Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 129 (1): 76–81. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30011.
PMID 15274045.
^ Ronald A, Happé F, Bolton P, et al (2006). "Genetic heterogeneity
between the three components of the autism spectrum: a twin study".
Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
45 (6): 691–9. doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000215325.13058.9d. PMID 16721319.
^ Joseph J (2006). "Autism and genetics: much ado about very
little", The Missing Gene: Psychiatry, Heredity, and the Fruitless
Search for Genes. Algora. ISBN 0875864104. Retrieved on 2007-07-25.
^ a b Bolton P, Macdonald H, Pickles A, et al (1994). "A case-control
family history study of autism". Journal of child psychology
and psychiatry, and allied disciplines 35 (5): 877–900. PMID 7962246.
^ Hughes C, Plumet MH, Leboyer M (1999). "Towards a cognitive
phenotype for autism: increased prevalence of executive dysfunction
and superior spatial span amongst siblings of children with autism".
Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines
40 (5): 705–18. PMID 10433405.
^ Lauritsen MB, Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB (2005). "Effects
of familial risk factors and place of birth on the risk of autism:
a nationwide register-based study". Journal of child psychology
and psychiatry, and allied disciplines 46 (9): 963–71. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00391.x.
PMID 16108999.
^ a b Zhao X, Leotta A, Kustanovich V, et al (2007). "A unified
genetic theory for sporadic and inherited autism". Proc. Natl.
Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104 (31): 12831–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.0705803104.
PMID 17652511. Lay summary – CSHL (2007-07-23).
^ Piven J, Wzorek M, Landa R, et al (1994). "Personality characteristics
of the parents of autistic individuals". Psychological medicine
24 (3): 783–95. PMID 7991760.
^ Piven J, Palmer P, Jacobi D, Childress D, Arndt S (1997). "Broader
autism phenotype: evidence from a family history study of multiple-incidence
autism families". The American journal of psychiatry 154 (2):
185–90. PMID 9016266.
^ Baron-Cohen S, Bolton P, Wheelwright S, et al. "Autism occurs
more often in families of physicists, engineers, and mathematicians".
(PDF} Autism, 1998, 2, 296-301. Retrieved December 10, 2006.
^ Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Stott C, et al. "Is there a
link between engineering and autism?" (PDF) Autism, 1997, 1,
153-163. Retrieved December 10, 2006.
^ Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S (2001). "The link between autism
and skills such as engineering, maths, physics and computing: a
reply to Jarrold and Routh". Autism : the international journal
of research and practice 5 (2): 223–7. PMID 11706868. Online.
^ Silberman, Steve. The Geek Syndrome. Wired Magazine (December
2001). Retrieved on December 10, 2006.
^ Happé F, Briskman J, Frith U (2001). "Exploring the cognitive
phenotype of autism: weak "central coherence" in parents
and siblings of children with autism: I. Experimental tests".
Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines
42 (3): 299–307. PMID 11321199.
^ Abramson RK, Ravan SA, Wright HH, et al (2005). "The relationship
between restrictive and repetitive behaviors in individuals with
autism and obsessive compulsive symptoms in parents". Child
psychiatry and human development 36 (2): 155–65. doi:10.1007/s10578-005-2973-7.
PMID 16228144.
^ Constantino JN, Todd RD (2005). "Intergenerational transmission
of subthreshold autistic traits in the general population".
Biol. Psychiatry 57 (6): 655–60. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.014.
PMID 15780853.
^ Ghaziuddin M (2005). "A family history study of Asperger
syndrome". Journal of autism and developmental disorders 35
(2): 177–82. PMID 15909404.
^ Greenberg DA, Hodge SE, Sowinski J, Nicoll D (2001). "Excess
of twins among affected sibling pairs with autism: implications
for the etiology of autism". Am. J. Hum. Genet. 69 (5): 1062–7.
PMID 11590546.
^ Hallmayer J, Glasson EJ, Bower C, et al (2002). "On the twin
risk in autism". Am. J. Hum. Genet. 71 (4): 941–6. PMID 12297988.
^ Freitag CM (2007). "The genetics of autistic disorders and
its clinical relevance: a review of the literature". Mol Psychiatry
12 (1): 2–22. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001896. PMID 17033636. Retrieved
on 2007-07-18.
^ Hobson RP, Lee A, Brown R (1999). "Autism and congenital
blindness". Journal of autism and developmental disorders 29
(1): 45–56. PMID 10097994.
^ Hoksbergen R, ter Laak J, Rijk K, van Dijkum C, Stoutjesdijk F
(2005). "Post-Institutional Autistic Syndrome in Romanian adoptees".
Journal of autism and developmental disorders 35 (5): 615–23. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0005-x.
PMID 16167089.
^ Rutter ML, Kreppner JM, O'Connor TG (2001). "Specificity
and heterogeneity in children's responses to profound institutional
privation". The British journal of psychiatry : the journal
of mental science 179: 97–103. PMID 11483469.
^ Rutter M, Bailey A, Bolton P, Le Couteur A (1994). "Autism
and known medical conditions: myth and substance". Journal
of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines 35 (2):
311–22. PMID 8188801.
^ Autism and Autism Spectrum Disorders Genetic origin: is there
an "autism" gene? WebPediatrics.com. Retrieved on March
2, 2007.
^ Pickles A, Bolton P, Macdonald H, et al (1995). "Latent-class
analysis of recurrence risks for complex phenotypes with selection
and measurement error: a twin and family history study of autism".
Am. J. Hum. Genet. 57 (3): 717–26. PMID 7668301.
^ Risch N, Spiker D, Lotspeich L, et al (1999). "A genomic
screen of autism: evidence for a multilocus etiology". Am.
J. Hum. Genet. 65 (2): 493–507. PMID 10417292.
^ Jiang YH, Sahoo T, Michaelis RC, et al (2004). "A mixed epigenetic/genetic
model for oligogenic inheritance of autism with a limited role for
UBE3A". Am. J. Med. Genet. A 131 (1): 1–10. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.30297.
PMID 15389703.
^ Skuse DH (2000). "Imprinting, the X-chromosome, and the male
brain: explaining sex differences in the liability to autism".
Pediatr. Res. 47 (1): 9–16. PMID 10625077.
^ Schanen NC (2006). "Epigenetics of Autism Spectrum Disorders".
Hum Mol Genet 15 (Review 2): R138–50. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddl213. PMID
16987877.
^ Surtees PG, Wainwright NW, Willis-Owen SA, Luben R, Day NE, Flint
J (2006). "Social adversity, the serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR)
polymorphism and major depressive disorder". Biol. Psychiatry
59 (3): 224–9. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.014. PMID 16154545.
^ Sutcliffe JS, Delahanty RJ, Prasad HC, et al (2005). "Allelic
heterogeneity at the serotonin transporter locus (SLC6A4) confers
susceptibility to autism and rigid-compulsive behaviors". Am.
J. Hum. Genet. 77 (2): 265–79. doi:10.1086/432648. PMID 15995945.
^ Devlin B, Cook EH, Coon H, et al (2005). "Autism and the
serotonin transporter: the long and short of it". Mol. Psychiatry
10 (12): 1110–6. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001724. PMID 16103890.
^ Coutinho AM, Oliveira G, Morgadinho T, et al (2004). "Variants
of the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4) significantly contribute
to hyperserotonemia in autism". Mol. Psychiatry 9 (3): 264–71.
doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001409. PMID 15094787.
^ Ma DQ, Whitehead PL, Menold MM, et al (2005). "Identification
of significant association and gene-gene interaction of GABA receptor
subunit genes in autism". Am. J. Hum. Genet. 77 (3): 377–88.
doi:10.1086/433195. PMID 16080114.
^ a b Nurmi EL, Dowd M, Tadevosyan-Leyfer O, Haines JL, Folstein
SE, Sutcliffe JS (2003). "Exploratory subsetting of autism
families based on savant skills improves evidence of genetic linkage
to 15q11-q13". Journal of the American Academy of Child and
Adolescent Psychiatry 42 (7): 856–63. doi:10.1097/01.CHI.0000046868.56865.0F.
PMID 12819446.
^ Delorey TM, Sahbaie P, Hashemi E, Homanics GE, Clark JD (2007).
"Gabrb3 gene deficient mice exhibit impaired social and exploratory
behaviors, deficits in non-selective attention and hypoplasia of
cerebellar vermal lobules: A potential model of Autism Spectrum Disorder". Behav Brain Res. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.09.009.
PMID 17983671.
^ Benayed R, Gharani N, Rossman I, et al (2005). "Support for
the homeobox transcription factor gene ENGRAILED 2 as an Autism Spectrum Disorder susceptibility locus". Am. J. Hum. Genet.
77 (5): 851–68. doi:10.1086/497705. PMID 16252243.
^ Zhong H, Serajee FJ, Nabi R, Huq AH (2003). "No association
between the EN2 gene and autistic disorder". J. Med. Genet.
40 (1): e4. PMID 12525552.
^ Auranen M, Varilo T, Alen R, et al (2003). "Evidence for
allelic association on chromosome 3q25-27 in families with Autism Spectrum Disorders originating from a subisolate of Finland".
Mol. Psychiatry 8 (10): 879–84. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001299. PMID
14515138.
^ Ylisaukko-oja T, Nieminen-von Wendt T, Kempas E, et al (2004).
"Genome-wide scan for loci of Asperger syndrome". Mol.
Psychiatry 9 (2): 161–8. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001385. PMID 14966474.
^ Auranen M, Vanhala R, Varilo T, et al (2002). "A genomewide
screen for autism-spectrum disorders: evidence for a major susceptibility
locus on chromosome 3q25-27". Am. J. Hum. Genet. 71 (4): 777–90.
PMID 12192642.
^ Serajee FJ, Zhong H, Mahbubul Huq AH (2006). "Association
of Reelin gene polymorphisms with autism". Genomics 87 (1):
75–83. doi:10.1016/j.ygeno.2005.09.008. PMID 16311013.
^ Skaar DA, Shao Y, Haines JL, et al (2005). "Analysis of the
RELN gene as a genetic risk factor for autism". Mol. Psychiatry
10 (6): 563–71. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001614. PMID 15558079.
^ Li J, Nguyen L, Gleason C, et al (2004). "Lack of evidence
for an association between WNT2 and RELN polymorphisms and autism".
Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 126 (1): 51–7. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.20122.
PMID 15048648.
^ Segurado R, Conroy J, Meally E, Fitzgerald M, Gill M, Gallagher
L (2005). "Confirmation of association between autism and the
mitochondrial aspartate/glutamate carrier SLC25A12 gene on chromosome
2q31". The American journal of psychiatry 162 (11): 2182–4.
doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.11.2182. PMID 16263864.
^ Ramoz N, Reichert JG, Smith CJ, et al (2004). "Linkage and
association of the mitochondrial aspartate/glutamate carrier SLC25A12
gene with autism". The American journal of psychiatry 161 (4):
662–9. PMID 15056512.
^ Blasi F, Bacchelli E, Carone S, et al (2006). "SLC25A12 and
CMYA3 gene variants are not associated with autism in the IMGSAC
multiplex family sample". Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 14 (1): 123–6.
doi:10.1038/sj.ejhg.5201444. PMID 16205742.
^ Kent L, Gallagher L, Elliot HR, Mowbray C, Chinnery PF (2007).
"An investigation of mitochondrial haplogroups in autism".
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30687.
PMID 18161860.
^ Rodier PM (2000). "The early origins of autism". Sci.
Am. 282 (2): 56–63. PMID 10710787.
^ Conciatori M, Stodgell CJ, Hyman SL, et al (2004). "Association
between the HOXA1 A218G polymorphism and increased head circumference
in patients with autism". Biol. Psychiatry 55 (4): 413–9. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.10.005.
PMID 14960295.
^ Gallagher L, Hawi Z, Kearney G, Fitzgerald M, Gill M (2004). "No
association between allelic variants of HOXA1/HOXB1 and autism".
Am. J. Med. Genet. B Neuropsychiatr. Genet. 124 (1): 64–7. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.20094.
PMID 14681917.
^ Collins JS, Schroer RJ, Bird J, Michaelis RC (2003). "The
HOXA1 A218G polymorphism and autism: lack of association in white
and black patients from the South Carolina Autism Project".
Journal of autism and developmental disorders 33 (3): 343–8. PMID
12908836.
^ Talebizadeh Z, Bittel DC, Miles JH, et al (2002). "No association
between HOXA1 and HOXB1 genes and Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)".
J. Med. Genet. 39 (11): e70. PMID 12414832.
^ Tischfield MA, Bosley TM, Salih MA, et al (2005). "Homozygous
HOXA1 mutations disrupt human brainstem, inner ear, cardiovascular
and cognitive development". Nat. Genet. 37 (10): 1035–7. doi:10.1038/ng1636.
PMID 16155570.
^ Ingram JL, Stodgell CJ, Hyman SL, Figlewicz DA, Weitkamp LR, Rodier
PM (2000). "Discovery of allelic variants of HOXA1 and HOXB1:
genetic susceptibility to Autism Spectrum Disorders". Teratology
62 (6): 393–405. doi:<393::AID-TERA6>3.0.CO;2-V 10.1002/1096-9926(200012)62:6<393::AID-TERA6>3.0.CO;2-V.
PMID 11091361.
^ Rossel M, Capecchi MR (1999). "Mice mutant for both Hoxa1
and Hoxb1 show extensive remodeling of the hindbrain and defects
in craniofacial development". Development 126 (22): 5027–40.
PMID 10529420.
^ Philippi A, Roschmann E, Tores F, et al (2005). "Haplotypes
in the gene encoding protein kinase c-beta (PRKCB1) on chromosome
16 are associated with autism". Mol. Psychiatry 10 (10): 950–60.
doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001704. PMID 16027742.
^ Marui T, Koishi S, Funatogawa I, et al (2005). "No association
of FOXP2 and PTPRZ1 on 7q31 with autism from the Japanese population".
Neurosci. Res. 53 (1): 91–4. doi:10.1016/j.neures.2005.05.003. PMID
15998549.
^ Gauthier J, Joober R, Mottron L, et al (2003). "Mutation
screening of FOXP2 in individuals diagnosed with autistic disorder".
Am. J. Med. Genet. A 118 (2): 172–5. doi:10.1002/ajmg.a.10105. PMID
12655497.
^ Samaco RC, Hogart A, LaSalle JM (2005). "Epigenetic overlap
in autism-spectrum neurodevelopmental disorders: MECP2 deficiency
causes reduced expression of UBE3A and GABRB3". Hum. Mol. Genet.
14 (4): 483–92. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddi045. PMID 15615769.
^ Schuetz G, Rosário M, Grimm J, Boeckers TM, Gundelfinger ED, Birchmeier
W (2004). "The neuronal scaffold protein Shank3 mediates signaling
and biological function of the receptor tyrosine kinase Ret in epithelial
cells". J. Cell Biol. 167 (5): 945–52. doi:10.1083/jcb.200404108.
PMID 15569713.
^ a b c Deletion 22q13 Syndrome M.C Phelan (2003) Orphanet.com
^ a b Durand CM, Betancur C, Boeckers TM, et al (2007). "Mutations
in the gene encoding the synaptic scaffolding protein SHANK3 are
associated with Autism Spectrum Disorders". Nat. Genet. 39
(1): 25–7. doi:10.1038/ng1933. PMID 17173049. Retrieved on 2007-11-07.
Lay summary – Autism Speaks.
^ Neuroligins Kristen Harris (2001) Cell adhesion at synapses Synapse
Web, Laboratory of Synapse Structure and Function. Human Brain Project.
National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institute of
Drug Abuse
^ Graf ER, Zhang X, Jin SX, Linhoff MW, Craig AM (2004). "Neurexins
induce differentiation of GABA and glutamate postsynaptic specializations
via neuroligins". Cell 119 (7): 1013–26. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2004.11.035.
PMID 15620359.
^ Tabuchi K, Blundell J, Etherton MR et al. (2007). "A neuroligin-3
mutation implicated in autism increases inhibitory synaptic transmission
in mice". Science 318 (5847): 71–6. doi:10.1126/science.1146221.
PMID 17823315. Lay summary – Science Daily (2007-09-08).
^ a b Gene Linked to Autism in Families with More Than One Affected
Child National Institutes of Health News (2006) Retrieved March
3, 2007
^ Campbell DB, Sutcliffe JS, Ebert PJ, et al (2006). "A genetic
variant that disrupts MET transcription is associated with autism".
Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103 (45): 16834–9. doi:10.1073/pnas.0605296103.
PMID 17053076. Lay summary – BBC News (2006-10-28).
^ Autism Genome Project Consortium (2007). "Mapping autism
risk loci using genetic linkage and chromosomal rearrangements".
Nat Genet 39 (3): 319–28. doi:10.1038/ng1985. PMID 17322880. Lay
summary – Press release (2007-02-18). Corrigendum (2007). Nat Genet
39 (10): 1285. doi:10.1038/ng1007-1285a. PMID 17898782.
^ Williams TA, Mars AE, Buyske SG, et al (2007). "Risk of autistic
disorder in affected offspring of mothers with a glutathione S-transferase
P1 haplotype". Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine
161 (4): 356–61. doi:10.1001/archpedi.161.4.356. PMID 17404132.
^ a b Ylisaukko-oja T, Alarcón M, Cantor RM, et al (2006). "Search
for autism loci by combined analysis of Autism Genetic Resource
Exchange and Finnish families". Ann. Neurol. 59 (1): 145–55.
doi:10.1002/ana.20722. PMID 16288458.
^ Lauritsen MB, Als TD, Dahl HA, et al (2006). "A genome-wide
search for alleles and haplotypes associated with autism and related
pervasive developmental disorders on the Faroe Islands". Mol.
Psychiatry 11 (1): 37–46. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001754. PMID 16205737.
^ Trikalinos TA, Karvouni A, Zintzaras E, et al (2006). "A
heterogeneity-based genome search meta-analysis for autism-spectrum
disorders". Mol. Psychiatry 11 (1): 29–36. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001750.
PMID 16189507.
^ Butler MG, Dasouki MJ, Zhou XP, et al (2005). "Subset of
individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorders and extreme macrocephaly
associated with germline PTEN tumour suppressor gene mutations".
J. Med. Genet. 42 (4): 318–21. doi:10.1136/jmg.2004.024646. PMID
15805158.
External links
Click here to go to the
home page to view the full range of autism fact sheets at www.autism-help.org
This autism fact sheet is licensed under the GNU
Free Documentation. It is derivative of an autism-related articles at http://en.wikipedia.org |