Fact sheet on diagnosis of Autism, an Autism Spectrum Disorder
 
 

CAUSES OF AUTISM

Autism and Autism Spectrum Disorders are complex neurodevelopmental disorders. Many causes of autism have been proposed, but its theory of causation is still incomplete.[1] Heritability contributes about 90% of the risk of a child developing autism, but the genetics of autism are complex and typically it is unclear which genes are responsible.[2] In rare cases, autism is strongly associated with agents that cause birth defects.[3] Many other causes have been proposed, such as exposure of children to vaccines; these proposals are controversial and the vaccine hypotheses have no convincing scientific evidence.[4]

 

Autism is a condition involving abnormalities of brain development and behavior which manifests itself before a child is three years old and has a steady course with no remission. It is characterized by impairments in social interaction and communication, as well as restricted and repetitive behaviors. It is part of a larger family called the Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) or pervasive developmental disorders (PDD), which include closely related syndromes such as Asperger syndrome and PDD-NOS.[5][6] This article uses autism to denote the classic autistic disorder and Autism Spectrum Disorder to denote the wider family.

 

Autism's theory of causation is still incomplete.[1] There is increasing suspicion among researchers that autism does not have a single cause, but is instead a complex disorder with a triad of core aspects (social impairment, communication difficulties, and repetitive behaviors) that have distinct causes but often co-occur.[7] The number of people known to have autism has increased dramatically since the 1980s, at least partly due to changes in diagnostic practice; it is unknown whether prevalence has increased as well.[8] An increase in prevalence would suggest directing more attention and funding toward changing environmental factors instead of continuing to focus on genetics. The consensus among mainstream autism researchers is that genetic factors predominate, but some are concerned, as one anonymous researcher put it, that "geneticists are running the show, and ignoring the environmental aspects."[9]

 

In 2007 the National Institutes of Health announced an Autism Centers of Excellence (ACE) research program to find the causes of autism and identify new treatments for the disorder. Initial recipients are focusing on genetic factors, brain imaging, brain chemicals and functions including mirror neurons, effect on early parent-child behavior on autism, and learning in autistic children.[10]

 

Genetics

Genetic factors are the most significant cause for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Early studies of twins estimated heritability to be over 90%, in other words, that genetics explains over 90% of whether a child will develop autism.[2] This may be an overestimate; new twin data and models with structural genetic variation are needed.[11] Many of the non-autistic co-twins had learning or social disabilities. For adult siblings the risk for having one or more features of the broader autism phenotype might be as high as 30%.[12]

 

The genetics of autism is complex.[2] Linkage analysis has been inconclusive; many association analyses have had inadequate power.[11] More than one gene may be implicated, different genes may be involved in different individuals, and the genes may interact with each other or with environmental factors. Several candidate genes have been located,[13] but the mutations that increase autism risk have not been identified for most candidate genes.

 

Though autism's genetic factors explain most of autism risk, they do not explain all of it. A common hypothesis is that autism is caused by the interaction of a genetic predisposition and an early environmental insult.[1] Several theories based on environmental factors have been proposed to address the remaining risk. Some of these theories focus on prenatal environmental factors, such as agents that cause birth defects; others focus on the environment after birth, such as children's diets.

 

A 2007 review of risk factors found associated parental characteristics that included advanced maternal age, advanced paternal age, and maternal place of birth outside Europe or North America. It is not known whether these associations reflect genetic, epigenetic, or environmental factors.[14]

 

Click here to read more about Genetics as a cause of autism.

 

Prenatal environment

The risk of autism is associated with several prenatal risk factors. Autism has been linked to birth defect agents acting during the first eight weeks from conception, though these cases are rare. Other potential prenatal environmental factors do not have convincing scientific evidence.

 

Teratogens

Teratogens are environmental agents that cause birth defects. Some agents that are known to cause other birth defects have also been found to be related to autism risk. These include exposure of the embryo to thalidomide, valproic acid, or misoprostol, or to rubella infection in the mother. These cases are rare.[9] Questions have also been raised whether ethanol (grain alcohol) increases autism risk, as part of fetal alcohol syndrome or alcohol-related birth defects, but current evidence is insufficient to determine whether autism risk is actually elevated with ethanol.[15] All known teratogens appear to act during the first eight weeks from conception, and though this does not exclude the possibility that autism can be initiated or affected later, it is strong evidence that autism arises very early in development.[3] Infection-associated immunological events in early pregnancy may affect neural development more than infections in late pregnancy, not only for autism, but also for other psychiatric disorders of presumed neurodevelopmental origin, notably schizophrenia.[16]

 

Pesticides

A 2007 study by the California Department of Public Health found that women in the first eight weeks of pregnancy who live near farm fields sprayed with the organochlorine pesticides dicofol and endosulfan are several times more likely to give birth to children with autism. The association appeared to increase with dose and decrease with distance from field site to residence. The study's findings suggest that on the order of 7% of autism cases in the California Central Valley might have been connected to exposure to the insecticides drifting off fields into residential areas. These results are highly preliminary due to the small number of women and children involved and lack of evidence from other studies.[17] It is not known whether these pesticides are human teratogens, though endosulfan has significant teratogenic effects in laboratory rats.[18]

 

A 2005 study showed indirect evidence that prenatal exposure to organophosphate pesticides such as diazinon and chlorpyrifos may contribute to autism in genetically vulnerable children.[19] Several other studies demonstrate the neurodevelopmental toxicity of these agents at relatively low exposure levels.[20]

 

Folic acid

Folic acid taken during pregnancy might play an important role in causing autism by modulating gene expression through epigenetic mechanism. This hypothesis is untested.[21]

 

Fetal testosterone

The fetal testosterone theory hypothesizes that higher levels of testosterone in the amniotic fluid of mothers pushes brain development towards improved ability to see patterns and analyze complex systems while diminishing communication and empathy, emphasizing "male" traits over "female", or in EQ SQ Theory terminology, emphasizing "systemizing" over "empathizing".[22] One project has published several reports suggesting that high levels of fetal testosterone could produce behaviors relevant to those seen in autism.[23] The theory and findings are controversial and many studies contradict the idea that baby boys and girls respond differently to people and objects.[24]

 

Ultrasound

A 2006 study found that sustained exposure of mouse embryos to ultrasound waves caused a small but statistically significant number of neurons to fail to acquire their proper position during neuronal migration.[25] It is highly unlikely that this result speaks directly to risks of fetal ultrasound as practiced in competent and responsible medical centers.[26] There is no scientific evidence of an association between prenatal ultrasound exposure and autism, but there are very little data on human fetal exposure during diagnostic ultrasound, and the lack of recent epidemiological research and human data in the field has been called "appalling".[27]

 

Perinatal environment

Autism is associated with some perinatal and obstetric conditions. A 2007 review of risk factors found associated obstetric conditions that included low birth weight and gestation duration, and hypoxia during childbirth. This association does not demonstrate a causal relationship; an underlying cause could explain both autism and these associated conditions.[14] A 2007 study of premature infants found that those who survived cerebellar hemorrhagic injury (bleeding in the brain that injures the cerebellum) were significantly more likely to show symptoms of autism than controls without the injury.[28]

 

Postnatal environment

A wide variety of postnatal contributors to autism have been proposed, including gastrointestinal or immune system abnormalities, allergies, and exposure of children to drugs, vaccines, infection, certain foods, or heavy metals. The evidence for these risk factors is anecdotal and has not been confirmed by reliable studies.[4] The subject remains controversial and extensive further searches for environmental factors are underway.[9]

 

Leaky gut syndrome

Parents have reported gastrointestinal (GI) disturbances in autistic children, and several studies have investigated possible associations between autism and the gut.[29] The controversial Wakefield et al. vaccine paper discussed in "MMR vaccine" below also suggested that some bowel disorders may allow antigens to pass from food into the bloodstream and then to contribute to brain dysfunction.[30] This produced several lines of investigation.

 

For example, employing secretin's effects on digestion, a 1998 study of three children with Autism Spectrum Disorder treated with secretin infusion reported improved GI function and dramatic improvement in behavior, which suggested an association between GI and brain function in autistic children.[31] After this study, many parents sought secretin treatment and a black market for the hormone developed quickly.[29] However, later studies found secretin ineffective in treating autism.[32]

 

Leaky gut syndrome theories also inspired several dietary treatments, including gluten-free diets, casein-free diets, antifungal diets, low-sugar diets, as well as supplements that include nystatin, B12, and probiotics. Parents are more likely to get advice about these diets from other parents, the media, and the Internet than from medical experts. There is no solid research evidence that autistic children are more likely to have GI symptoms than typical children.[29] In particular, design flaws in studies of elimination diets mean that the currently available data are inadequate to guide treatment recommendations.[33]

 

Viral infection

Many studies have presented evidence for and against association of autism with viral infection after birth. Laboratory rats infected with Borna disease virus show some symptoms similar to those of autism but blood studies of autistic children show no evidence of infection by this virus. Members of the herpes virus family may have a role in autism, but the evidence so far is anecdotal. Viruses have long been suspected as triggers for immune-mediated diseases such as multiple sclerosis but showing a direct role for viral causation is difficult in those diseases, and mechanisms whereby viral infections could lead to autism are speculative.[34]

 

Oxidative stress

This theory hypothesizes that toxicity and oxidative stress may cause autism in some cases by damaging Purkinje cells in the cerebellum after birth. One possibility is that glutathione is involved.[35]

 

Amygdala neurons

This theory hypothesizes that an early developmental failure involving the amygdala cascades on the development of cortical areas that mediate social perception in the visual domain. The fusiform face area of the ventral stream is implicated. The idea is that it is involved in social knowledge and social cognition, and that the deficits in this network are instrumental in causing autism.[36]

 

Vitamin D

This theory hypothesizes that autism is caused by vitamin D deficiency, and that recent increases in diagnosed cases of autism are due to medical advice to avoid the sun. The theory has not been studied scientifically.[37]

 

Lead

Lead poisoning has been suggested as a possible risk factor for autism, as the lead blood levels of autistic children has been reported to be significantly higher than typical. The atypical eating behaviors of autistic children, along with habitual mouthing and pica, make it hard to determine whether increased lead levels are a cause or a consequence of autism.

 

Mercury

This theory hypothesizes that autism is associated with mercury poisoning, based on perceived similarity of symptoms.[39] The principal source of human exposure to organic mercury is via fish consumption and for inorganic mercury is dental amalgams. Other forms of exposure, such as in cosmetics and vaccines, also occur. The evidence so far is indirect for the association between autism and mercury exposure after birth, as no direct test has been reported, and there is no evidence of an association between autism and postnatal exposure to any neurotoxicant.[40]

 

A 2003 study reported that mercury measurements of hair samples from autistic children's first haircuts were significantly lower than a matched group of normal children, declining as measures of severity increased,[41] but a later meta-analysis based on two studies found that there was not enough evidence to conclude that hair mercury level is lower in autistic children.[42] A 2006 study found an association between autism and environmental releases of mercury, primarily from coal power plants; this study used Texas county-wide data and did not distinguish between prenatal and postnatal exposure.[43]

 

Perhaps the best-known theory involving mercury and autism involves the use of the mercury-based compound thiomersal, a preservative that has been phased out from most childhood vaccinations in developed countries. Parents may first become aware of autistic symptoms in their child around the time of a routine vaccination. There is no convincing scientific evidence for a causal connection between thiomersal and autism, but parental concern about the thiomersal controversy has led to decreasing uptake of childhood immunizations and increasing likelihood of disease outbreaks.[44]

 

MMR vaccine

The MMR vaccine theory of autism is one of the most extensively debated theories regarding the origins of autism. A controversial 1998 paper by Andrew Wakefield et al. reported a study of 12 children who had autism and bowel symptoms, in some cases reportedly with onset after MMR. Though the paper concluded "We did not prove an association between measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine and the syndrome described,"[30] Wakefield nevertheless suggested during a 1998 press conference that giving children the vaccines in three separate doses would be safer than a single jab. This suggestion was again not supported by the paper, his co-authors or by any scientific evidence[45] and has been heavily criticized, both on scientific grounds and for triggering a decline in vaccination rates.[44] Using separate, single vaccines in place of MMR is widely believed to put children at increased risk since the combined vaccine reduces the risk of them catching the diseases while they are waiting for full immunization cover.[46] Numerous peer-reviewed studies have also since failed to show any association between MMR vaccine and autism.[44]

 

In 2004, the interpretation of a causal link between MMR vaccine and autism was formally retracted by ten of Wakefield's twelve co-authors.[47] The retraction followed an investigation by The Sunday Times.[48] The Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,[49] the Institute of Medicine of the National Academy of Sciences,[50] and the U.K. National Health Service[51] have all concluded that there is no evidence of a link between the MMR vaccine and autism.

 

In July 2007 Andrew Wakefield and coauthors John Walker-Smith and Simon Murch faced charges of serious professional misconduct at the General Medical Council. It is alleged that the trio acted unethically in preparing the research into safety of the MMR vaccine. Wakefield denies the charges.[52]

 

Television watching

Three economists hypothesized that early childhood television viewing acts as an environmental trigger for an underlying genetic predisposition. They found that precipitation was associated with autism by examining county-level autism data for California, Oregon, and Washington. Precipitation is also associated with television watching, and their analysis concluded that just under 40% of autism diagnoses in the three states result from television watching due to precipitation.[53] This study has not been published in a refereed journal and its results have not been confirmed by others.[54]

 

Refrigerator mother

Bruno Bettelheim believed that autism was linked to early childhood trauma, and his work was highly influential for decades both in the medical and popular spheres. Parents, especially mothers, of individuals with autism were blamed for having caused their child's condition through the withholding of affection.[55] Leo Kanner, who first described autism,[56] suggested that parental coldness might contribute to autism.[57] Although Kanner eventually renounced the theory, Bettelheim put an almost exclusive emphasis on it in both his medical and his popular books. Treatments based on these theories failed to help children with autism, and after Bettelheim's death it came out that his reported rates of cure (around 85%) were found to be fraudulent.[58]

 

Other psychogenic theories

Psychogenic theories in general have become increasingly unpopular, particularly since twin studies have shown that autism is highly heritable. Nevertheless, some case reports have found that deep institutional privation can result in "quasi-autistic" features without the neuroanatomical differences.[59][60] Other case reports have suggested that children predisposed genetically to autism can develop "autistic devices" in response to traumatic events such as the birth of a sibling.[61]

 

Social construct

Like ADHD, which has a similar social construct theory,[62] a spectral disorder such as autism may be understood as a cultural or social construct.[63] The theory says that the boundary between normal and abnormal is subjective and arbitrary, so autism does not exist as an objective entity, but only as a social construct. It further argues that autistic individuals themselves have a way of being that is partly socially constructed.[64] This theory does not say that there are no neurological or quality-of-life differences between groups deemed "autistic" and "non-autistic". To falsify this theory it would need to be shown that an objective characteristic can clearly separate both groups. For example, a genetic test that can fully substitute for a psychiatric diagnosis would undermine this theory.[65]Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism are particular targets of the theory that social factors determine what it means to be autistic. The theory hypothesizes that individuals with these diagnoses inhabit the identities that have been ascribed to them, and promote their sense of well-being by resisting or appropriating autistic ascriptions.[66]

 

the mechanism of autism

Despite extensive investigation, how autism occurs is not well understood. Its mechanism can be divided into two areas: the pathophysiology of brain structures and processes associated with autism, and the neuropsychological linkages between brain structures and behaviors.[9] The behaviors appear to have multiple pathophysiologies.[23]

 

Pathophysiology

Autism appears to result from developmental factors that affect many or all functional brain systems.[49] Neuroanatomical studies and the associations with teratogens strongly suggest that autism's mechanism includes alteration of brain development soon after conception.[2] This localized anomaly appears to start a cascade of pathological events in the brain that are significantly influenced by environmental factors.[58] Many major structures of the human brain have been implicated. Consistent abnormalities have been found in the development of the cerebral cortex; and in the cerebellum and related inferior olive, which have a significant decrease in the number of Purkinje cells. Brain weight and volume and head circumference tend to be greater in autistic children.[59] The cellular and molecular bases of pathological early overgrowth are not known, nor is it known whether the overgrown neural systems cause autism's characteristic signs. Current hypotheses include:

 

* An excess of neurons that causes local overconnectivity in key brain regions.[60]
* Disturbed neuronal migration during early gestation.[47][61]
* Unbalanced excitatory-inhibitory networks.[47]
* Abnormal formation of synapses and dendritic spines.[47]

 

Interactions between the immune system and the nervous system begin early during embryogenesis, and successful neurodevelopment depends on a balanced immune response. Several symptoms consistent with a poorly regulated immune response have been reported in autistic children. It is possible that aberrant immune activity during critical periods of neurodevelopment is part of the mechanism of some forms of Autism Spectrum Disorder.[62] As autoantibodies have not been associated with pathology, are found in diseases other than Autism Spectrum Disorder, and are not always present in ASD,[63] the relationship between immune disturbances and autism remains unclear and controversial.[61]

 

Several neurotransmitter abnormalities have been detected in autism, notably increased blood levels of serotonin. Whether these lead to structural or behavioral abnormalities is unclear.[9]

 

The mirror neuron system (MNS) theory of autism hypothesizes that distortion in the development of the MNS interferes with imitation and leads to autism's core features of social impairment and communication difficulties. The MNS operates when an animal performs an action or observes another animal of the same species perform the same action. The MNS may contribute to an individual's understanding of other people by enabling the modeling of their behavior via embodied simulation of their actions, intentions, and emotions.[64] Several studies have tested this hypothesis by demonstrating structural abnormalities in MNS regions of individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder, delay in the activation in the core circuit for imitation in individuals with Asperger's, and a correlation between reduced MNS activity and severity of the syndrome in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder.[65] A variant theory, EP-M, segments the MNS into an indirect route for goal emulation and planning (EP) and a direct route for mimicry (M), and hypothesizes that only the M route is impaired in autism.[66]

 

The underconnectivity theory of autism hypothesizes that autism is marked by underfunctioning high-level neural connections and synchronization, along with an excess of low-level processes. Evidence for this theory has been found in functional neuroimaging studies on autistic individuals[33] and by a brain wave study that suggested that adults with Autism Spectrum Disorder have local overconnectivity in the cortex and weak functional connections between the frontal lobe and the rest of the cortex. Other evidence suggests the underconnectivity is mainly within each hemisphere of the cortex and that autism is a disorder of the association cortex.[69]

 

Neuropsychology

Two major categories of cognitive theories have been proposed about the links between autistic brains and behavior.

 

The first category focuses on deficits in social cognition. Hyper-systemizing hypothesizes that autistic individuals can systematize—that is, they can develop internal rules of operation to handle internal events—but are less effective at empathizing by handling events generated by other agents.[17] It extends the extreme male brain theory, which hypothesizes that autism is an extreme case of the male brain, defined psychometrically as individuals in whom systemizing is better than empathizing. This in turn is related to the earlier theory of mind, which hypothesizes that autistic behavior arises from an inability to ascribe mental states to oneself and others. The theory of mind is supported by autistic children's atypical responses to the Sally-Anne test for reasoning about others' motivations, and is mapped well from the mirror neuron system theory of autism.[65]

 

The second category focuses on nonsocial or general processing. Executive dysfunction hypothesizes that autistic behavior results in part from deficits in flexibility, planning, and other forms of executive function. A strength of the theory is predicting stereotyped behavior and narrow interests; a weakness is that executive function deficits are not found in young autistic children.[27] Weak central coherence theory hypothesizes that a limited ability to see the big picture underlies the central disturbance in autism. One strength of this theory is predicting special talents and peaks in performance in autistic people. A related theory—enhanced perceptual functioning—focuses more on the superiority of locally oriented and perceptual operations in autistic individuals. These theories map well from the underconnectivity theory of autism.

 

Neither category is satisfactory on its own; social cognition theories poorly address autism's rigid and repetitive behaviors, while the nonsocial theories have difficulty explaining social impairment and communication difficulties.[43] A combined theory based on multiple deficits may prove to be more useful.[7]

 

References

1. ^ a b c Trottier G, Srivastava L, Walker CD (1999). "Etiology of infantile autism: a review of recent advances in genetic and neurobiological research". J Psychiatry Neurosci 24 (2): 103–115. PMID 10212552. Retrieved on 2007-07-16.
2. ^ a b c Freitag CM (2007). "The genetics of autistic disorders and its clinical relevance: a review of the literature". Mol Psychiatry 12 (1): 2–22. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001896. PMID 17033636.
3. ^ a b Arndt TL, Stodgell CJ, Rodier PM (2005). "The teratology of autism". Int J Dev Neurosci 23 (2–3): 189–99. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.11.001. PMID 15749245.
4. ^ a b Rutter M (2005). "Incidence of Autism Spectrum Disorders: changes over time and their meaning". Acta Paediatr 94 (1): 2–15. PMID 15858952.
5. ^ American Psychiatric Association (2000). "Diagnostic criteria for 299.00 Autistic Disorder", Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed., text revision (DSM-IV-TR). ISBN 0890420254.
6. ^ World Health Organization (2006). "F84. Pervasive developmental disorders", International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 10th ed. (ICD-10).
7. ^ Happé F, Ronald A, Plomin R (2006). "Time to give up on a single explanation for autism". Nat Neurosci 9 (10): 1218–20. doi:10.1038/nn1770. PMID 17001340.
8. ^ Newschaffer CJ, Croen LA, Daniels J et al. (2007). "The epidemiology of Autism Spectrum Disorders". Annu Rev Public Health 28: 235–58. doi:10.1146/annurev.publhealth.28.021406.144007. PMID 17367287.
9. ^ a b c Szpir M (2006). "Tracing the origins of autism: a spectrum of new studies". Environ Health Perspect 114 (7): A412–8. PMID 16835042.
10. ^ National Institutes of Health (2007-08-02). NIH funds new program to investigate causes and treatment of autism. Press release. Retrieved on 2007-08-03.
11. ^ a b Sykes NH, Lamb JA (2007). "Autism: the quest for the genes". Expert Rev Mol Med 9 (24): 1–15. doi:10.1017/S1462399407000452. PMID 17764594.
12. ^ Folstein SE, Rosen-Sheidley B (2001). "Genetics of autism: complex aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder". Nat Rev Genet 2 (12): 943–55. doi:10.1038/35103559. PMID 11733747.
13. ^ Persico AM, Bourgeron T (2006). "Searching for ways out of the autism maze: genetic, epigenetic and environmental clues". Trends Neurosci 29 (7): 349–58. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2006.05.010. PMID 16808981.
14. ^ a b Kolevzon A, Gross R, Reichenberg A (2007). "Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for autism". Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 161 (4): 326–33. PMID 17404128.
15. ^ Fombonne E (2002). "Is exposure to alcohol during pregnancy a risk factor for autism?". J Autism Dev Disord 32 (3): 243. doi:10.1023/A:1015466100838. PMID 12108626.
16. ^ Meyer U, Yee BK, Feldon J (2007). "The neurodevelopmental impact of prenatal infections at different times of pregnancy: the earlier the worse?". Neuroscientist 13 (3): 241–56.. doi:10.1177/1073858406296401. PMID 17519367.
17. ^ Roberts EM, English PB, Grether JK, Windham GC, Somberg L, Wolff C (2007). "Maternal residence near agricultural pesticide applications and Autism Spectrum Disorders among children in the California Central Valley". Environ Health Perspect 115 (10): 1482–9. doi:10.1289/ehp.10168. PMID 17938740. Lay summary – EHP (2007).
18. ^ Singh ND, Sharma AK, Dwivedi P, Patil RD, Kumar M (2007). "Citrinin and endosulfan induced teratogenic effects in Wistar rats". J Appl Toxicol 27 (2): 143–51. doi:10.1002/jat.1185. PMID 17186572.
19. ^ D'Amelio M, Ricci I, Sacco R et al. (2005). "Paraoxonase gene variants are associated with autism in North America, but not in Italy: possible regional specificity in gene-environment interactions". Mol Psychiatry 10 (11): 1006–16. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001714. PMID 16027737.
20. ^ Karr CJ, Solomon GM, Brock-Utne AC (2007). "Health effects of common home, lawn, and garden pesticides". Pediatr Clin North Am 54 (1): 63–80. doi:10.1016/j.pcl.2006.11.005. PMID 17306684.
21. ^ Muskiet FA, Kemperman RF (2006). "Folate and long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in psychiatric disease". J Nutr Biochem 17 (11): 717–27. doi:10.1016/j.jnutbio.2006.02.001. PMID 16650750.
22. ^ Baron-Cohen S (2004). The Essential Difference: Male and Female Brains and the Truth About Autism. Basic Books. ISBN 046500556X.
23. ^ Fetal testosterone studies:
* Knickmeyer RC, Baron-Cohen S (2006). "Fetal testosterone and sex differences". Early Hum Dev 82 (12): 755–60. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2006.09.014. PMID 17084045.
* Morelle R. "Hormone linked to autistic traits", BBC, 2007-09-11. Retrieved on 2007-09-11.
24. ^ Rivers C (2006-09-28). Discrimination against the female brain. AlterNet. Retrieved on 2006-12-10.
25. ^ Ang ES Jr, Gluncic V, Duque A, Schafer ME, Rakic P (2006). "Prenatal exposure to ultrasound waves impacts neuronal migration in mice". Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103 (34): 12903–10. doi:10.1073/pnas.0605294103. PMID 16901978.
26. ^ Caviness VS, Grant PE (2006). "Our unborn children at risk?". Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 103 (34): 12661–2. doi:10.1073/pnas.0605505103.
27. ^ Abramowicz JS (2007). "Prenatal exposure to ultrasound waves: is there a risk?". Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol 29 (4): 363–7. doi:10.1002/uog.3983. PMID 17352453.
28. ^ Limperopoulos C, Bassan H, Gauvreau K et al. (2007). "Does cerebellar injury in premature infants contribute to the high prevalence of long-term cognitive, learning, and behavioral disability in survivors?". Pediatrics 120 (3): 584–93. doi:10.1542/peds.2007-1041. PMID 17766532.
29. ^ a b c Johnson TW (2006). "Dietary considerations in autism: identifying a reasonable approach". Top Clin Nutr 21 (3): 212–25.
30. ^ a b Wakefield A, Murch S, Anthony A et al. (1998). "Ileal-lymphoid-nodular hyperplasia, non-specific colitis, and pervasive developmental disorder in children". Lancet 351 (9103): 637–41. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(97)11096-0. PMID 9500320. Retrieved on 2007-09-05.
31. ^ Horvath K, Stefanatos G, Sokolski KN, Wachtel R, Nabors L, Tildon JT (1998). "Improved social and language skills after secretin administration in patients with autism spectrum disorders". J Assoc Acad Minor Phys 9 (1): 9–15. PMID 9585670.
32. ^ Sturmey P (2005). "Secretin is an ineffective treatment for pervasive developmental disabilities: a review of 15 double-blind randomized controlled trials". Res Dev Disabil 26 (1): 87–97. PMID 15590241.
33. ^ Christison GW, Ivany K (2006). "Elimination diets in Autism Spectrum Disorders: any wheat amidst the chaff?". J Dev Behav Pediatr 27 (2 Suppl 2): S162–71. PMID 16685183.
34. ^ Libbey JE, Sweeten TL, McMahon WM, Fujinami RS (2005). "Autistic disorder and viral infections". J Neurovirol 11 (1): 1–10. doi:10.1080/13550280590900553. PMID 15804954.
35. ^ Kern JK, Jones AM (2006). "Evidence of toxicity, oxidative stress, and neuronal insult in autism". J Toxicol Environ Health B Crit Rev 9 (6): 485–99.. doi:10.1080/10937400600882079. PMID 17090484.
36. ^ Schultz RT (2005). "Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area". Int J Dev Neurosci 23 (2–3): 125–41. doi:10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.12.012. PMID 15749240.
37. ^ Cannell JJ (2007). "Autsim and vitamin D". Med Hypotheses. PMID 17920208.
38. ^ Zafeiriou DI, Ververi A, Vargiami E (2007). "Childhood autism and associated comorbidities". Brain Dev 29 (5): 257–72. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2006.09.003. PMID 17084999.
39. ^ Bernard S, Enayati A, Redwood L, Roger H, Binstock T (2001). "Autism: a novel form of mercury poisoning". Med Hypotheses 56 (4): 462–71. doi:10.1054/mehy.2000.1281. PMID 11339848.
40. ^ Davidson PW, Myers GJ, Weiss B (2004). "Mercury exposure and child development outcomes". Pediatrics 113 (4 Suppl): 1023–9. PMID 15060195.
41. ^ Holmes AS, Blaxill MF, Haley BE (2003). "Reduced levels of mercury in first baby haircuts of autistic children". Int J Toxicol 22 (4): 277–85. PMID 12933322.
42. ^ Ng DK, Chan CH, Soo MT, Lee RS (2007). "Low-level chronic mercury exposure in children and adolescents: meta-analysis". Pediatr Int 49 (1): 80–7. doi:10.1111/j.1442-200X.2007.02303.x. PMID 17250511.
43. ^ Palmer RF, Blanchard S, Stein Z, Mandell D, Miller C (2006). "Environmental mercury release, special education rates, and autism disorder: an ecological study of Texas". Health Place 12 (2): 203–9. doi:10.1016/j.healthplace.2004.11.005. PMID 16338635.
44. ^ a b c Doja A, Roberts W (2006). "Immunizations and autism: a review of the literature". Can J Neurol Sci 33 (4): 341–6. PMID 17168158.
45. ^ MMR – the controversy. UK Prime Minister's Office (2005). Retrieved on 2007-07-29.
46. ^ MMR the facts. NHS. Retrieved on 2007-07-29.
47. ^ Murch SH, Anthony A, Casson DH et al. (2004). "Retraction of an interpretation". Lancet 363 (9411): 750. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(04)15715-2. PMID 15016483.
48. ^ Deer B (2007). The MMR-autism scare – our story so far. Retrieved on 2007-07-29.
49. ^ Autism and vaccines theory. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (2007-07-05). Retrieved on 2007-07-29.
50. ^ Immunization safety review: vaccines and autism. Institute of Medicine, National Academy of Sciences (2004). Retrieved on 2007-06-13.
51. ^ MMR the facts. National Health Service. Retrieved on 2007-06-13.
52. ^ "MMR scare doctor 'paid children'", BBC News, 2007-07-16. Retrieved on 2007-07-29.
53. ^ Waldman M, Nicholson S, Adliov N (2006). "Does television cause autism?". Johnson School Research Paper Series No. 01-07. Retrieved on 2007-07-27.
54. ^ Wallis C. "Does watching TV cause autism?", TIME, 2006-10-20. Retrieved on 2007-07-28.
55. ^ Bettelheim B (1967). The Empty Fortress: Infantile Autism and the Birth of the Self. Free Press. ISBN 0029031400.
56. ^ Kanner L (1943). "Autistic disturbances of affective contact". Nerv Child 2: 217–50.
57. ^ Kanner L (1949). "Problems of nosology and psychodynamics in early childhood autism". Am J Orthopsychiatry 19: 416–26.
58. ^ Gardner M (2000). "The brutality of Dr. Bettelheim". Skeptical Inquirer 24 (6): 12–4.
59. ^ Rutter ML, Kreppner JM, O'Connor TG, English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) study team (2001). "Specificity and heterogeneity in children's responses to profound institutional privation". Br J Psychiatry 179 (2): 97–103. PMID 11483469.
60. ^ Hoksbergen R, ter Laak J, Rijk K, van Dijkum C, Stoutjesdijk F (2005). "Post-Institutional Autistic Syndrome in Romanian adoptees". J Autism Dev Disord 35 (5): 615–23. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0005-x. PMID 16167089.
61. ^ Gomberoff M, De Gomberoff LP (2000). "Autistic devices in small children in mourning". Int J Psychoanal 81 (5): 907–20. PMID 11109576.
62. ^ Timimi S, Taylor E (2004). "ADHD is best understood as a cultural construct". Br J Psychiatry 184: 8–9. PMID 14702221.
63. ^ Timimi S (2004). "Diagnosis of autism: current epidemic has social context". BMJ 328 (7433): 226. doi:10.1136/bmj.328.7433.226-a. PMID 14739199.
64. ^ Hacking I (1999). The Social Construction of What?. Harvard University Press, 114–23. ISBN 0674004124.
65. ^ Does autism exist?. Natural Variation – Autism Blog (2006-02-24). Retrieved on 2007-07-29.
66. ^ Nadesan MH (2005). "The dialectics of autism: theorizing autism, performing autism, remediating autism, and resisting autism", Constructing Autism: Unravelling the 'Truth' and Understanding the Social. Routledge, 179. ISBN 0415321816.

Click to close this Autism fact sheet on causes

Click here for the full range of autism and Asperger's fact sheets at www.autism-help.org
This autism fact sheet is licensed under the GNU Free Documentation. It is derivative of autism and Aspergers--related articles at http://en.wikipedia.org

   
   
The possible causes of Autism and Aspergers syndrome usually revolve around both genetic and environmental factors